Skip to main content
Open Book Publishers

3. Charting a middle course: Theory and methods in the practice of cross-cultural research

  • Ivan Kroupin (author)
  • Felix Reide (author)
  • April Nowell (author)
  • Chantal Medaets (author)
  • Mark Nielsen (contributions by)
  • Ana Maria R. Gomes (contributions by)
  • Gairan Pamei (contributions by)
  • Patricia M. Greenfield (contributions by)
  • Andrea Taverna (contributions by)
  • Andrew D. Coppens (contributions by)
  • Akira Takada (contributions by)
  • Miguel Silan (contributions by)
  • Heidi Keller (contributions by)
  • Kara Weisman (contributions by)
  • Bruno Ferreira (contributions by)

Export Metadata

  • ONIX 3.1
  • ONIX 3.0
    • Thoth
    • Project MUSE
      Cannot generate record: No BIC or BISAC subject code
    • OAPEN
    • JSTOR
      Cannot generate record: No BISAC subject code
    • Google Books
      Cannot generate record: No BIC, BISAC or LCC subject code
    • OverDrive
      Cannot generate record: No priced EPUB or PDF URL
  • ONIX 2.1
  • CSV
  • JSON
  • OCLC KBART
  • BibTeX
  • CrossRef DOI deposit
    Cannot generate record: This work does not have any ISBNs
  • MARC 21 Record
    Cannot generate record: MARC records are not available for chapters
  • MARC 21 Markup
    Cannot generate record: MARC records are not available for chapters
  • MARC 21 XML
    Cannot generate record: MARC records are not available for chapters
Metadata
Title3. Charting a middle course
SubtitleTheory and methods in the practice of cross-cultural research
ContributorIvan Kroupin (author)
Felix Reide (author)
April Nowell (author)
Chantal Medaets (author)
Mark Nielsen (contributions by)
Ana Maria R. Gomes (contributions by)
Gairan Pamei (contributions by)
Patricia M. Greenfield (contributions by)
Andrea Taverna (contributions by)
Andrew D. Coppens (contributions by)
Akira Takada (contributions by)
Miguel Silan (contributions by)
Heidi Keller (contributions by)
Kara Weisman (contributions by)
Bruno Ferreira (contributions by)
DOIhttps://doi.org/10.11647/obp.0440.03
Landing pagehttps://www.openbookpublishers.com/books/10.11647/obp.0440/chapters/10.11647/obp.0440.03
Licensehttps://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/
CopyrightIvan Kroupin; Felix Riede; April Nowell; Chantal Medaets;
PublisherOpen Book Publishers
Published on2025-05-09
Long abstractRecent years have seen a resurgence in work arguing for the importance of cross-cultural research. Yet, there are few guides and worked examples of how theory in cognitive science and anthropology can actually be instantiated in a productive research program. This chapter collects contributions on this topic, with several background essays on the practice of cross cultural research and six concrete examples of research programs. Across these contributions, the recurring theme is balancing the need for generating generalizable science with attention to local cultural contexts. Instead of converging on a single solution, these contributions provide a lay of the land, demonstrating the various ways in which researchers have found a pragmatic balance between the universal and the specific in studying our cultural species.
Page rangepp. 67–110
Print length44 pages
LanguageEnglish (Original)
Contributors

Ivan Kroupin

(author)
Postdoctoral researcher in the Department of Psychological and Behavioural Science at The London School of Economics and Political Science
Postdoctoral researcher in the Department of Human Evolutionary Biology at Harvard University

Ivan Kroupin is a postdoctoral researcher in the Department of Psychological and Behavioural Science, London School of Economics and Political Science, UK, and the Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, US. As co-director of the Ecology of Mind project (Kunene region—Namibia/Angola), Ivan focuses on how urbanization and technology are reshaping our minds and wellbeing—and the evolutionary dynamics driving this transformation. This perspective informs and is informed by an interdisciplinary theoretical framework, integrating ideas and data from developmental biology, systems theory, cultural evolution, and cognitive science.

Felix Reide

(author)
Professor of Archaeology in the Department of Archaeology and Heritage Studies at Aarhus University

Felix Riede is Professor of Archaeology in the Department of Archaeology and Heritage Studies, Aarhus University, Denmark. He works in a broad and inclusive cultural evolutionary framework. Focused on understanding the interactions between social learning, niche construction, and environmental change, he explores the role of children in human cognitive evolution, and the role of object play as a motor for material culture variation, innovation, and adaptation.

April Nowell

(author)
Professor of Anthropology at University of Victoria

April Nowell is a Paleolithic archaeologist and Professor of Anthropology at the University of Victoria, Canada. She specializes in the origins of art, language and modern cognition, Neanderthal lifeways, and the lives of children and adolescents in the Pleistocene. She is the author of Growing Up in the Ice Age, winner of the 2023 European Association of Archaeologists book prize.

Chantal Medaets

(author)
Associate Professor of Anthropology at Universidade Estadual de Campinas

Chantal Medaets is Associate Professor of Anthropology at the School of Education at the State University of Campinas (Unicamp), Brazil, where she coordinates the Anthropology and Education Research Center (Ceape). She holds a Master’s degree in Education (2009) and a PhD in Social Anthropology (2015) both from Paris Descartes University (since 2022, Paris Cité University). She has carried out research on Indigenous and river dweller childhoods and forms of socialization in the Lower Tapajós region in the Brazilian Amazon, analyzing parenting styles, informal education (out of school transmission and learning practices) and its connections to the school. In her current research project, she deals with different aspects of the Indigenous presence in Higher Education in Brazil, combining ethnographic fieldwork at Unicamp with the analysis of intercultural educational politics for Higher Education at a national level

Mark Nielsen

(contributions by)
Professor of Developmental Psychology at University of Queensland
Senior Research Associate at University of Johannesburg

Mark Nielsen is a Professor of Developmental Psychology at the University of Queensland, Australia, and a Senior Research Associate at the University of Johannesburg. His research interests lie in a range of inter-related aspects of socio-cognitive development in young human children and non-human primates. His current research is primarily focused on charting the origins and development of human cultural cognition.

Ana Maria R. Gomes

(contributions by)
Titular Professor at the Faculty of Education at Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais

Ana Maria Gomes is Titular Professor at the Faculty of Education, Federal University of Minas Gerais, Brazil. She holds a PhD in Education from the University of Bologna, Italy (1996). She was a postdoctoral researcher in Social Anthropology at the Museu Nacional-UFRJ, Brazil (2008), and in the Department of Anthropology at the University of St. Andrews, UK (2017). She has carried out f ield research with children in Brazilian metropolises and Italian Roma children. Since 2000, she has been working with Indigenous Peoples in different Brazilian States as a CNPq (National Research Council) researcher in the field of Anthropology and Education, mainly on the topics of Indigenous intercultural education, culture and schooling, learning and culture, cosmopolitics, and ecology of practices.

Gairan Pamei

(contributions by)

Gairan Pamei is a final year PhD candidate in the Department of Psychology, the Chinese University of Hong Kong, researching literacies across educational-linguistic contexts. Her work is informed by developmental science of child learning and methodological advances in psychometrics.

Patricia M. Greenfield

(contributions by)
Distinguished Professor of Psychology at University of California, Los Angeles

Patricia Greenfield is Distinguished Professor of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, US. She earned her PhD from the interdisciplinary Department of Social Relations at Harvard. Her dissertation focused on a cross-cultural study of culture and cognitive development in Senegal. Collaborating with international researchers, she has published cross-cultural and intergenerational studies on learning, socialization, and human development in Maya communities, Mexico; among Bedouin, Northern Arab, and Ethiopian immigrant populations in Israel; and within dominant majority groups in Burma/Myanmar, Romania, Japan, China, and Turkey. In the United States, she and her collaborators have conducted research on these topics among European Americans, Asian Americans, and immigrant groups from Mexico, Central America, and Korea.

Andrea Taverna

(contributions by)
Researcher at Instituto Rosario de Investigaciones en Ciencias de la Educación (IRICE) at National Scientific and Technical Research Council

Andrea Taverna holds a PhD in psychology and works as a researcher at the Instituto Rosario de Investigaciones en Ciencias de la Educación (IRICE), Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET), Universidad Nacional de Rosario (UNR), Argentina. Her current interest is the study of the acquisition of Wichi, an Indigenous language spoken in northern Argentina, as a mother tongue. She is currently describing the early grammaticalization process of complex morphology and the socialization context in which this ancestral language emerges. She is a group leader and since 2010 she and her students have been conducting fieldwork in the Wichi communities in collaboration with Indigenous teachers and community leaders.

Andrew D. Coppens

(contributions by)
Associate Professor in the Department of Education at University of New Hampshire

Andrew D. Coppens is Associate Professor in the Department of Education, University of New Hampshire, US. Andrew Coppens’ research focuses on cultural processes of informal learning and development in family and community contexts. He has conducted research on young children’s development of collaborative helping with rural, middle-class, and Indigenous-heritage communities in the US, Mexico, Ecuador, Germany, India, and Bhutan. Coppens is founding PI of the Youth Retention Initiative, a research collaborative focused on developing sustainable and strengths based educational and workforce pathways to address human capital extraction patterns among rural communities.

Akira Takada

(contributions by)
Professor in the Graduate School of Asian and African Area Studies at Kyoto University

Akira Takada is Professor in the Graduate School of Asian and African Area Studies at Kyoto University, Japan. He has worked with/among groups of the San of southern Africa (particularly !Xun and ǂAkhoe in Namibia, G|ui and Gǁana in Botswana) since the late 1990s. He has published a number of books and articles, including Hunters Among Farmers: The !Xun of Ekoka (2022), and The Ecology of Playful Childhood: The Diversity and Resilience of Caregiver-Child Interactions among the San of Southern Africa (2020).

Miguel Silan

(contributions by)
co-founder and Chief Behavioral Strategist at Lumière University Lyon 2

Miguel Silan is the co-founder and Chief Behavioral Strategist in the Annecy Behavioral Science Lab, France. His work involves evaluating how behavioral science methods work and fail, and how to improve them, especially for vulnerable populations. He is also an Associate Director for the Psychological Science Accelerator, a network of more than 2,000 researchers across 84 countries, and is helping advance new ways of conducting large-scale collaborations in psychology.

Heidi Keller

(contributions by)
Professor Emeritus of Human Sciences at Osnabrück University

Heidi Keller, Professor Emeritus of Human Sciences at University of Osnabrück, Germany, is currently a Distinguished Fellow of the Paul Baerwald School of Social Work and Social Welfare at the Hebrew University, Jerusalem. Besides cross-cultural development research on early child development, she is interested in the scientific and ethical problems of applying basic research to different fields of practice.

Kara Weisman

(contributions by)
Postdoctoral Project Director of the Developing Belief Network, Department of Psychology at University of California, Riverside

Kara Weisman is the Postdoctoral Project Director of the Developing Belief Network, Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, US. She is a cognitive scientist and developmental psychologist with roots in psychology, anthropology, and philosophy. She received her PhD in psychology from Stanford University in 2019, followed by postdoctoral roles on two large scale, international collaborations in cultural anthropology and developmental science. Her work focuses on folk theories and their role in shaping people’s behaviors, relationships, and experiences.

Bruno Ferreira

(contributions by)
Associate Professor at the Faculty of Education at Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul

Bruno Ferreira holds a Bachelor’s degree in History, Full Teaching License from the Regional University of the Northwest of the State of Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil (1999), a Master’s degree in Education from the Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS) (2014), and a PhD in Education from the Faculty of Education at the same university (2020). He is an Associate Professor at the Faculty of Education, UFRGS, and a researcher in the Peabiru Research Group: Amerindian Education and Interculturality (CNPq/UFRGS). His research focuses on Indigenous Methodologies, Indigenous intellectuals, Indigenous school education, and intercultural education.

References
  1. Abu Aleon, T., Weinstock, M., Manago, A. M., & Greenfield, P. M. (2019). Social change and intergenerational value differences in a Bedouin community in Israel. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 50, 708–727. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022119839148
  2. Amir, D., & McAuliffe, K. (2020). Cross-cultural, developmental psychology: Integrating approaches and key insights. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(5), 430–444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.06.006
  3. Arleo, A., & Delalande, J. (2010). Cultures enfantines: Universalité et diversité. Presses universitaires de Rennes. https://doi.org/10.4000/books.pur.10732
  4. Avineri, N., Johnson, E., Brice-Heath, S., McCarty, T., Ochs, E., Kremer-Sadlik, T., Blum, S., Zentella, A. C., Rosa, J., Flores, N., Alim, H. S., & Paris, D. (2015). Invited forum: Bridging the “language gap”. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 25(1). https://doi.org/10.1111/jola.12071
  5. Baiocchi, M., Waxman, S., Pérez, E., Pérez, A., & Taverna, A. (2019). Social-ecological relations among animals serve as a conceptual framework among the Wichi. Cognitive Development, 52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2019.100807.
  6. Bakhurst, D. (2009). Reflections on activity theory. Educational Review, 61(2), 197–210. https://doi.org/10.1080/00131910902846916
  7. Bakker, M., & Wicherts, J. M. (2011). The (mis)reporting of statistical results in psychology journals. Behavior Research Methods, 43(3), 666–678. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0089-5
  8. Baumard, N., & Sperber, D. (2010). Weird people, yes, but also weird experiments. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3). https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10000038
  9. Bensa, A. (1995). De la relation ethnographique. Enquête, 1, 131–140. https://doi.org/10.4000/enquete.268
  10. Bergamaschi, M. A. (2008). Povos Indígenas & Educação. Mediação.
  11. Bian, Q., Chen, Y., Greenfield, P. M., & Yuan, Q. (2022). Mothers’ Experience of Social Change and Individualistic Parenting Goals Over Two Generations in Urban China. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.487039
  12. Bruner, J. (1990). Culture and Human Development: A New Look. Human Development, 33(6), 344–355. https://doi.org/10/fk2tqp
  13. Burman, J. T. (2020). On Kuhn’s case, and Piaget’s: A critical two-sited hauntology (or, On impact without reference. History of the Human Sciences, 33(3–4), 129–159. https://doi.org/10.1177/0952695120911576
  14. Burman, J. T. (2022). Meaning-change through the mistaken mirror: On the indeterminacy of “Wundt” and “Piaget ( in Translation., Trans.). Review of General Psychology, 26(1), 22–48. https://doi.org/10.1177/10892680211017521
  15. Cohn, C. (2000). A criança indígena: A concepção Xikrin de infância e aprendizado. University of São Paulo. https://doi.org/10.11606/D.8.2000.tde-15042024-142639
  16. Cohn, C. (2017). Les dessins d’enfants et l’anthropologie: Une étude chez les Xicrin (Pará, Brésil. In A. Pierrot, I. M. Carvalho & C. Medaets (Eds). Domination et apprentissage. Anthropologie des formes de la transmission culturelle. Hermann. https://doi.org/10.4000/rfp.6745
  17. Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline (pp. xvi, 400). Harvard University Press.
  18. Cole, M., Gay, J., Glick, J., & Sharp, D. W. (1971). The Cultural context of learning and thinking: An exploration in experimental anthropology. Basic Books.
  19. Cole, M., Hood, L., & McDermott, R. (1978). Ecological niche picking: Ecological invalidity as an axiom of experimental cognitive psychology. LCHC and ICHD, Rockefeller University. https://doi.org/10.13140/2.1.4727.1204
  20. Collaboration, O. S. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science (New York, N.Y.), 349(6251), 4716. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4716
  21. Corsaro, W. A. (2003). We’re friends, right?: Inside kids’ culture. Joseph Henry Press.
  22. D’Andrade, R. (2000). The Sad Story of Anthropology 1950-1999. Cross-Cultural Research, 34(3), 219–232. https://doi.org/10.1177/106939710003400301
  23. Deffner, D., Rohrer, J. M., & McElreath, R. (2022). A Causal Framework for Cross-Cultural Generalizability. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 5(3), 25152459221106366. https://doi.org/10.1177/25152459221106366
  24. Di Paolo, E., & De Jaegher, H. (2016). Neither individualistic, nor interactionist. In C. Durt, T. Fuchs, C. Tewes, & enaction (Eds). Embodiment, enaction, and culture: Investigating the constitution of the shared world (pp. 87–105). MIT Press.
  25. Doria, N. G., & Simão, L. M. (2018). Differing times and differing measures: Dimensions of historical time in Vygotsky’s work. Theory & Psychology, 28(6), 757–779. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354318787345
  26. Draper, C. E., Barnett, L. M., Cook, C. J., Cuartas, J. A., Howard, S. J., McCoy, D. C., & Yousafzai, A. K. (2022). Publishing child development research from around the world: An unfair playing field resulting in most of the world’s child population under-represented in research. Infant and Child Development, e2375. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2375
  27. Duranti, A., Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. B. (2012). The handbook of language socialization. Wiley-Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444342901
  28. Ellis, B. D., & Stam, H. J. (2015). Crisis? What crisis? Cross-cultural psychology’s appropriation of cultural psychology. Culture & Psychology, 21(3), 293–317. https://doi.org/10.1177/1354067X15601198
  29. El-sana, S., Greenfield, P., & Weinstock, M. (2023). Ecological change, psychological mindedness, and attitudes toward school psychology: A three-generation study of Bedouin women in Israel. Applied Developmental Science, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2023.2192498
  30. Esteban-Guitart, M. (2018). The biosocial foundation of the early Vygotsky: Educational psychology before the zone of proximal development. History of Psychology, 21(4), 384–401. https://doi.org/10.1037/hop0000092
  31. Evers, N. F. G., Evers, G. W., Greenfield, P. M., Yuan, Q., Gutierrez, F., Halim, G., & Du, H. (2024). COVID-19 increased mortality salience, collectivism, and subsistence activities. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 55(3), 239-259. https://doi.org/10.1177/00220221231226310
  32. Evers, N. F. G., Greenfield, P. M., & Evers, G. W. (2021). COVID-19 shifts mortality salience, activities, and values in the United States: Big data analysis of online adaptation. Human Behavior and Emerging Technologies. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbe2.251
  33. Feldman-Barrett, L. (2017). How emotions are made: The secret life of the brain. Pan Macmillan.
  34. Fernández Ruiz, M., & Taverna, A. (2023). Native ontological framework guides causal reasoning: Evidence from Wichi people. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 23(3–4), 397–419. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685373-12340169
  35. García, C., Greenfield, P., Montiel-Acevedo, D., Vidaña-Rivera, T., & Colorado, J. (2017). Implications of 43 Years of Sociodemographic Change in Mexico for the Socialization of Achievement Behavior: Two Quasi-Experiments. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 48, 002202211769857. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022117698573
  36. García, C., Greenfield, P., Navarro, A., Colorado-García, J., & Vidaña-Rivera, T. (2020). Cooperative Play and Globalized Social Change: Mexican Children are Less Cooperative in 2017 than in 1967. Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology, 2, 100003. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cresp.2020.100003
  37. García, C., Rivera, N., & Greenfield, P. M. (2015). The decline of cooperation, the rise of competition: Developmental effects of long-term social change in Mexico. International Journal of Psychology, 50(1), 6–11. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12120
  38. Geertz, C. (1973). The Interpretation of Cultures. Basic Books.
  39. Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin.
  40. Goffman, E. (1964). The neglected situation. American Anthropologist, 66(6), 133–136. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1964.66.suppl_3.02a00090
  41. Gomes, A. M. R. (1998). Vegna che ta fago scriver. Etnografia della scolarizzazione in una comunità di sinti. CISU.
  42. Greenfield, P. M. (1966). On culture and conservation. In J. S. Bruner, R. R. Olver, & P. M. Greenfield (Eds). Studies in cognitive growth (pp. 225–256). Wiley.
  43. Greenfield, P. M. (1997). You can’t take it with you: Why ability assessments don’t cross cultures. American Psychologist, 52(10), 1115–1124. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.52.10.1115
  44. Greenfield, P. M. (2004). Weaving generations together: Evolving creativity in the maya of chiapas. School of American Research Press.
  45. Greenfield, P. M. (2009). Linking social change and developmental change: Shifting pathways of human development. Developmental Psychology, 45(2), 401–418. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014726
  46. Greenfield, P. M. (2016). Social change, cultural evolution, and human development. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 84–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.10.012
  47. Greenfield, P. M. (2018). Studying social change, culture, and human development: A theoretical framework and methodological guidelines. Developmental Review, 50, 16–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.05.003
  48. Greenfield, P. M., Brown, G., & Du, H. (2021). Shifts in ecology, values, behavior, and relationships during the coronavirus pandemic: Survival threat, subsistence activities, conservation of resources, and interdependent families. Current Research in Ecological Psychology, 2. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cresp.2021.100017
  49. Greenfield, P. M., & Bruner, J. S. (1966). Culture and Cognitive Growth. International Journal of Psychology, 1(2), 89–107. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207596608247117
  50. Greenfield, P. M., & Childs, C. P. (1977). Weaving, color terms, and pattern representation: Cultural influences and cognitive development among the Zinacantecos of Southern Mexico. Inter-American Journal of Psychology, 11, 23–48.
  51. Greenfield, P. M., & Quiroz, B. (2013). Context and culture in the socialization and development of personal achievement values: Comparing Latino immigrant families, European American families, and elementary school teachers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 34(2), 108–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2012.11.002
  52. Hammersley, M., & Atkinson, P. (2007). Ethnography: Principles in practice. Routledge
  53. Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life, and work in communities and classrooms. Cambridge University Press.
  54. Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
  55. Hruschka, D. J., Munira, S., Jesmin, K., Hackman, J., & Tiokhin, L. (2018). Learning from failures of protocol in cross-cultural research. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(45), 11428–11434. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1721166115
  56. Ionescu, A., Furdui, R., Gavreliuc, A., Greenfield, P. M., & Weinstock, M. (2023). The effects of sociocultural changes on epistemic thinking across three generations in Romania. PLOS ONE, 18(3), e0281785. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0281785
  57. Jolad, S., & Agarwal, A. (2021). Mapping india’s language and mother tongue diversity and its exclusion in the indian census. https://doi.org/10.31235/osf.io/sjxc6
  58. Keller, H. (2002). Development as the interface between biology and culture: A conceptualization of early ontogenetic experiences. In Between culture and biology: Perspectives on ontogenetic development (pp. 215–240). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511489853.011
  59. Keller, H. (2007). Cultures of Infancy. Erlbaum.
  60. Keller, H. (2022). Cultures of infancy. Routledge classis series. Routledge.
  61. Keller, H., & Bard, K. A. (Eds). (2017). The cultural nature of attachment: Contextualizing relationships and development. MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262036900.001.0001
  62. Keller, H., & Kärtner, J. (2013). Development The Cultural Solution of Universal Developmental Tasks. In M. J. Gelfand, C. Chiu, & Y. Hong (Eds.), Advances in Culture and Psychology (Volume 3. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199930449.001.0001
  63. Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75-86,. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4102_1
  64. Kronenfeld, D. B. (2017). Culture as a system: How we know the meaning and significance of what we do and say. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315267326
  65. Kroupin, I., Davis, H. E., & Henrich, J. (2024). Beyond Newton: Why Assumptions of Universality are Critical to Cognitive Science, and How to Finally Move Past Them. Psychological Review. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000480
  66. Lakatos, I. (1978). The methodology of scientific research programmes. Cambridge University Press. Press.https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511621123
  67. Lancy, D. F. (1981). The Indigenous Mathematics Project: An Overview. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 12(4), 445–453. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00308142
  68. LeVine, R. (2010). The six cultures study: Prologue to a history of a landmark project. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41(4), 513–521. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022110362567
  69. Levinson, S. C. (2012). The Original Sin of Cognitive Science. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(3), 396–403. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01195.x
  70. Lignier, W. (2019). The discovery of symbolic violence: How toddlers learn to prevail with words. Ethnography, 22(2), 246–266. https://doi.org/10.1177/1466138119872522
  71. Luhrmann, T. M., Weisman, K., Aulino, F., Brahinsky, J. D., Dulin, J. C., Dzokoto, V. A., Legare, C. H., Lifshitz, M., Ng., E., Ross-Zehnder, N., & Smith, R. E. (2021). Sensing the presence of gods and spirits across cultures and faiths. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(5), 2016649118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2016649118
  72. Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations. Harvard University Press.
  73. Manago, A. M. (2014). Connecting societal change to value differences across generations: Adolescents, mothers, and grandmothers in a Maya community in Southern Mexico. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45(6), 868–887. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022114527346
  74. Maynard, A. E., & Greenfield, P. M. (2003). Implicit cognitive development in cultural tools and children. Cognitive Development, 18, 489–510. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2003.09.005
  75. Maynard, A. E., Greenfield, P. M., & Childs, C. P. (2015). Developmental effects of economic and educational change: Cognitive representation across 43 years in a Maya community. International Journal of Psychology, 50, 12–19. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12129
  76. Maynard, A. E., Greenfield, P. M., Childs, C. P., & Weinstock, M. (2023). Social change, cultural evolution, weaving apprenticeship, and development: Informal education across three generations and 42 years in a Maya community. Applied Developmental Science. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2022.2151445
  77. Mead, M. (1932). An investigation of the thought of primitive children, with special reference to animism. The Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland, 62, 173–190. https://doi.org/10.2307/2843884
  78. Medaets, C. (2016). Despite adults: Learning experiences on the tapajós river banks. Ethos (Berkeley, Calif.), 44(3), 248–268. https://doi.org/10.1111/etho.12134
  79. Medin, D. L., Ojalehto, B., Marin, A., & Bang, M. (2013). Culture and epistemologies: Putting culture back into the ecosystem. In Y. Hong, M. J. Gelfand, & C. Chiu (Eds.), Advances in culture and psychology (vol. 4, pp. 177–217). Oxford University Press.
  80. Medin, D. L., Ojalehto, B., Waxman, S. R., & Bang, M. (2015). Relations: Language, epistemologies, categories, and concepts. In E. Margolis & S. Laurence (Eds). The conceptual mind: New directions in the study of concepts (pp. 349–378). MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9383.001.0001
  81. Medin, D., Ojalehto, B., Marin, A., & Bang, M. (2017). Systems of (non-) diversity. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(5), 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0088
  82. Miller, P. J., & Sperry, D. E. (2012). Déjà Vu: The continuing misrecognition of low-income children’s verbal abilities. In S. T. Fiske & H. R. Markus (Eds). Facing social class How societal rank influences interaction (1–6, pp. 109–130). Russell Sage Foundation.
  83. Morelli, C. (2023). Children of the rainforest: Shaping the future in amazonia. Rutgers University Press.
  84. Muthukrishna, M., Henrich, J., & Slingerland, E. (2021). Psychology as a Historical Science. Annual Review of Psychology, 72(1), 717–749. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-082820-111436
  85. Nielsen, M., & Haun, D. (2016). Why developmental psychology is incomplete without comparative and cross-cultural perspectives. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1686), 20150071. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0071
  86. Nielsen, M., Haun, D., Kärtner, J., & Legare, C. H. (2017). The persistent sampling bias in developmental psychology: A call to action. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 162, 31–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.04.017
  87. Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. B. (2012). The theory of language socialization. In A. Duranti, E. Ochs, & B. B. Schieffelin (Eds). The handbook of language socialization (p. 21). Wiley-Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444342901
  88. Overton, W. F. (2013a). A new paradigm for developmental science: Relationism and relational-developmental-systems. Applied Developmental Science, 17(2), 94–107. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2013.778717
  89. Overton, W. F. (2013b). Relationism and relational-developmental systems: A paradigm for developmental science in the post-Cartesian era. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 44, 21–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-397947-6.00002-7
  90. Overton, W. F., & Lerner, R. M. (2012). Relational developmental systems: A paradigm for developmental science in the postgenomic era. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(5), 375–376. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12001082
  91. Park, H., Twenge, J., & Greenfield, P. M. (2017). American undergraduate students’ value development during the Great Recession. International Journal of Psychology, 52, 28–39. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12410
  92. Park, H., Twenge, J. M., & Greenfield, P. M. (2014). The Great Recession: Implications for Adolescent Values and Behavior. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5(3), 310–318. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550613495419
  93. Peters, U., Krauss, A., & Braganza, O. (2022). Generalization Bias in Science. Cognitive Science, 46(9), e13188. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.13188
  94. Piaget, J. (1928). Judgment and reasoning in the child. Harcourt Brace.
  95. Pires, F., & Ribeiro, F. B. (2015). Crianças: Um enfoque geracional. Política & Trabalho, 43, 13–17.
  96. Rad, M. S., Martingano, A. J., & Ginges, J. (2018). Toward a psychology of Homo sapiens: Making psychological science more representative of the human population. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(45), 11401–11405. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1721165115
  97. Raeff, C., Greenfield, P. M., & Quiroz, B. (2000). Conceptualizing interpersonal relationships in the cultural contexts of individualism and collectivism. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2000(87), 59–74. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.23220008706
  98. Ramstead, M. J. D., Veissiere, S. P. L., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2016). Cultural affordances: Scaffolding local worlds through shared intentionality and regimes of attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01090
  99. Richert, R. A., Weisman, K., Lesage, K. A., Ghossainy, M. E., Reyes-Jaquez, B., & Corriveau, K. H. (2022). Belief, culture, & development: Insights from studying the development of religious beliefs and behaviors. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 62, 127–158. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.acdb.2021.11.002
  100. Rockwell, E. (2009). La experiencia etnográfica: Historia y cultura en los procesos educativos. Paidós.
  101. Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford University Press.
  102. Rogoff, B., & Chavajay, P. (1995). What’s Become of Research on the Cultural Basis of Cognitive Development? American Psychologist, 19. https://doi.org/10/ckxthb
  103. Rohrer, J. (2018). Thinking clearly about correlations and causation: Graphical causal models for observational research. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1, 27–42. https://doi.org/10.1177/25152459177456
  104. Rotem, O. S., Weinstock, M., & Greenfield, P. M. (2024). Changes in values and ways of knowing among three generations of Israeli women of Ethiopian origin. Current Research in Ecological Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cresp.2024.100186
  105. Sarcinelli, A. S. (2021). Des gamins roms hors-de-l’enfance. Entre protection et exclusion. Éd. des Archives contemporaines. https://doi.org/10.17184/eac.9782813003881
  106. Scheidecker, G., Chaudhary, N., Keller, H., Mezzenzana, F., & Lancy, D. (2023). “Poor brain development” in the global South? Challenging the science of early childhood interventions. Ethos, 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/etho.12379
  107. Schmidt, W. J., Keller, H., & Rosabal-Coto, M. (2021). Development in context: What we need to know to assess children’s attachment relationships. Developmental Psychology, 57(12), 2206–2219. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001262
  108. Silan, M. (2023). Rethinking multi-site studies: Can the cross-indigenous approach mitigate common cross-cultural vulnerabilities? In In press social and personality psychology compass. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/jsyca
  109. Szokolszky, A., & Read, C. (2018). Developmental ecological psychology and a coalition of ecological–relational developmental approaches. Ecological Psychology, 30(1), 6–38. https://doi.org/10.1080/10407413.2018.1410409
  110. Takada, A. (2012). Pre-verbal infant-caregiver interaction. In A. Duranti, E. Ochs, & B. B. Schieffelin (Eds). The handbook of language socialization (pp. 56–80). Wiley-Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444342901
  111. Takada, A. (2016). Education and learning during social situations among the Central Kalahari San. In H. Terashima & B. S. Hewlett (Eds). Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers: Evolutionary and ethnographic perspectives (pp. 97–111). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-55997-9_1
  112. Takada, A. (2019). Anthropology of interaction: Places where “mind” meets “culture.” Shinyosha.
  113. Tassinari, A. I. (2007). Concepções indígenas de infância no Brasil. Tellus, 7(13), 11–25.
  114. Taverna, A. (2021). Motherese in the Wichi Language (El maternés en la lengua wichí. Journal for the Study of Education and Development, 44(2), 303–335. https://doi.org/10.1080/02103702.2021.1889290
  115. Taverna, A., Padilla, M., Fernandez Ruiz, M., & Baiocchi, M. C. (2022). Concepts, language, and early socialization in the indigenous wichi perspective: Toward a relational–ecological paradigm. In M. V. Alves, R. Ekuni, M. J. Hermida, & J. Valle-Lisboa (Eds). Cognitive science and education in non-weird populations: A latin american perspective (pp. 74–97). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-06908-6
  116. Taverna, A., Padilla, M., & Waxman, S. (2024). How pervasive is joint attention? Mother-child dyads from a Wichi community reveal a different form of “togetherness.” Developmental Science. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13471
  117. Taverna, A., & Waxman, S. (2020). Early lexical acquisition in the Wichi language. Journal of Child Language, 47(5), 1052–1072. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000898
  118. Thurmond, V. A. (2001). The point of triangulation. Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 33(3), 253–258. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1547-5069.2001.00253.x
  119. Toren, C. (2011). The stuff of imagination: What we can learn from Fijian children’s ideas about their lives as adults. Social Analysis, 1, 23. https://doi.org/10.3167/sa.2011.550102
  120. Valsiner, J. (1998). The guided mind: A sociogenetic approach to personality. Harvard University Press.
  121. van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Poortinga, Y. H. (2016). On item pools, swimming pools, birds with webbed feet, and the professionalization of multilingual assessment. In Educational measurement: From foundations to future (pp. 273–290). The Guilford Press.
  122. Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes (M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman, Eds). Harvard University Press.
  123. Vygotsky, L. S. (1981). The genesis of higher mental functions. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.). The concept of activity in soviet psychology. M. F. Sharpe Inc Publisher.
  124. Vygotsky, L. S. (1998). The collected works of L.S. Vygotsky (vol. 5, R. W. Rieber Ed.). Springer.
  125. Vygotsky, L. S. (2012). Thought and Language. MIT Press.
  126. Weinstock, M. (2015). Changing epistemologies under conditions of social change in two Arab communities in Israel. International Journal of Psychology, 50(1), 29–36. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12130
  127. Weinstock, M., Ganayiem, M., Igbaryia, R., Manago, A. M., & Greenfield, P. M. (2015). Societal Change and Values in Arab Communities in Israel: Intergenerational and Rural–Urban Comparisons. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46(1), 19–38. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022114551792
  128. Weisman, K., et al. (2024). The development and diversity of religious cognition and behavior: Protocol for Wave 1 data collection with children and parents by the Developing Belief Network. Plos one, 19(3), e0292755. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292755
  129. Weisman, K., Legare, C. H., Smith, R. E., Dzokoto, V. A., Aulino, F., Ng, E., Dulin, J. D., Ross-Zehnder, N., Brahinsky, J. D., & Luhrmann, T. M. (2021). Similarities and differences in concepts of mental life among adults and children in five cultures. Nature Human Behaviour, 5, 1358–1368. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01184-8
  130. Weisman, K., & Luhrmann, T. M. (2020). What anthropologists can learn from psychologists, and the other way around. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 26(S1), 131–147. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9655.13245
  131. Wood, D. J., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychiatry and Psychology, 17, 89–100. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1976.tb00381.x
  132. Zeng, R., & Greenfield, P. M. (2015). Cultural evolution over the last 40 years in China: Using the Google Ngram Viewer to study implications of social and political change for cultural values. International Journal of Psychology, 50(1), 47–55. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12125
  133. Zhou, C., Yiu, W. Y. V., Wu, M. S., & Greenfield, P. M. (2017). Perception of cross-generational differences in child behavior and parent socialization: A mixed-method interview study with grandmothers in China. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 49, 62–81. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022117736029