4. Research Methods: A collage
- Elena Miu (author)
- Michelle Kline (contributions by)
- Ilaria Pretelli (contributions by)
- Bruce Rawlings (contributions by)
- Katja Liebal (contributions by)
- David F. Lancy (contributions by)
- Jing Xu (contributions by)
- Heejung Park (contributions by)
- Annemieke Milks (contributions by)
- Felix Reide (contributions by)
- Roman Stengelin (contributions by)
- Akira Takada (contributions by)
- Joscha Kärtner (contributions by)
Export Metadata
- ONIX 3.1
- ONIX 3.0
- ONIX 2.1
- CSV
- JSON
- OCLC KBART
- BibTeX
- CrossRef DOI depositCannot generate record: This work does not have any ISBNs
- MARC 21 RecordCannot generate record: MARC records are not available for chapters
- MARC 21 MarkupCannot generate record: MARC records are not available for chapters
- MARC 21 XMLCannot generate record: MARC records are not available for chapters
Title | 4. Research Methods |
---|---|
Subtitle | A collage |
Contributor | Elena Miu (author) |
Michelle Kline (contributions by) | |
Ilaria Pretelli (contributions by) | |
Bruce Rawlings (contributions by) | |
Katja Liebal (contributions by) | |
David F. Lancy (contributions by) | |
Jing Xu (contributions by) | |
Heejung Park (contributions by) | |
Annemieke Milks (contributions by) | |
Felix Reide (contributions by) | |
Roman Stengelin (contributions by) | |
Akira Takada (contributions by) | |
Joscha Kärtner (contributions by) | |
DOI | https://doi.org/10.11647/obp.0440.04 |
Landing page | https://www.openbookpublishers.com/books/10.11647/obp.0440/chapters/10.11647/obp.0440.04 |
License | https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/ |
Copyright | Elena Miu; |
Publisher | Open Book Publishers |
Published on | 2025-05-09 |
Long abstract | This chapter provides a non-comprehensive overview of the research methods used in cross-cultural studies of children. We present a collage of case studies and methods summaries that outline common practices and explore their theoretical, ethical, and practical implications. We first propose guidelines for dealing with issues of theoretical validity by showcasing the benefits of multi-methods perspectives for knowledge triangulation and introducing a design strategy for causal analysis. We cover practical organizational concerns pertinent to large cross-cultural studies. We then touch on ethical concerns and cover the value of long term cumulative study, revisiting old work from a non- western centric perspective, intersubjectivity and positionality, and include methods for sustainable and principled anthropology. Finally, we zoom in on methodological approaches used in cross-cultural work on children, summarizing the benefits and limitations of ethno-archaeological approaches, cross-cultural experiments, conversational analysis approaches, and standardized longitudinal assessments. |
Page range | pp. 111–162 |
Print length | 52 pages |
Language | English (Original) |
Elena Miu
(author)Elena Miu is an evolutionary biologist and anthropologist who uses empirical and theoretical modelling approaches to understand the population-level processes that underlie cultural adaptation, cultural change, and innovation. She is currently a postdoctoral researcher in the Department of Archaeology and Heritage Studies at Aarhus University, working on the effect of children’s play and exploration on innovation.
Michelle Kline
(contributions by)Michelle Kline is a Senior Lecturer of Psychology at Brunel University of London, UK. Her work focuses on social learning, including the evolution of teaching, how people learn to be parents, and the learning and creation of present-day spiritual beliefs. Her f ieldwork has included research in the Yasawa Islands, Fiji and in the town of Glastonbury, England.
Ilaria Pretelli
(contributions by)Ilaria Pretelli is an evolutionary biologist and anthropologist based at the Institute for Advanced Study in Toulouse, Toulouse School of Economics & University of Toulouse Capitole, France. She is interested in the evolution of human life history, with a focus on the emergence of childhood, learning, and cooperative breeding. She carried out her doctoral studies at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology in Leipzig, Germany. She has done f ieldwork on the island of Pemba, Zanzibar. Her research combines cross-cultural, statistical, and observational methods to study the evolution of human behavior.
Bruce Rawlings
(contributions by)Bruce Rawlings is a developmental, cross-cultural and comparative psychologist, based at Durham University, UK. He holds a PhD in Evolutionary Anthropology from the same university. His work examines cognitive and cultural influences on innovation, creativity, and tool use in children and great apes. He focuses on children across geographically and culturally diverse populations to understand what makes humans so unique. He also works to improve the validity of cross-cultural experiments
Katja Liebal
(contributions by)Katja Liebal is the head of the Human Biology and Primate Cognition group at the Faculty of Life Sciences, Institute of Biology, Leipzig University, Germany. She is a comparative psychologist with a background in behavioral biology. She is interested in the development and cross-cultural variability of children’s attitudes toward other animals. She cooperates with local collaborators from a variety of communities and uses a mixed-method approach, combining qualitative and quantitative methods from anthropology and psychology, to study human-animal relationships across a range of socio-cultural contexts. She has conducted fieldwork in Namibia, Zambia and Indonesia (including research with nonhuman primates).
David F. Lancy
(contributions by)David Lancy is Emeritus Professor of Anthropology at Utah State University, US. He has conducted extensive cross-cultural f ieldwork with children in interior Liberia, multiple sites in Papua New Guinea, Trinidad, Madagascar, Uganda, and Yemen, among others. More specifically, David Lancy has contributed to a better understanding of childhood-related topics including delayed personhood, the chore curriculum, children as a reserve labor force, children growing up in a gerontocracy, how children acquire their culture, socio-historical analyses of schooling, and the culture of street kids. He has written a survey of the field—recently published in a third edition—The Anthropology of Childhood: Cherubs, Chattel, Changelings. In total, David Lancy has authored nine books and edited three. His most recent work is Learning Without Lessons: Pedagogy in Indigenous Communities.
Jing Xu
(contributions by)Jing Xu is a Research Scientist at the Department of Anthropology, University of Washington, US. She is a cultural and cognitive anthropologist studying how culture and mind interact to shape child development in diverse geographic regions, historical periods and cross-cultural comparative contexts. She pursues interdisciplinary research, bringing together ethnography, experimental techniques, computational methods, and humanistic perspectives to study how humans become moral persons. She is the author of two monographs: The Good Child: Moral Development in a Chinese Preschool (2017) and “Unruly” Children: Historical Fieldnotes and Learning Morality in a Taiwan Village (2024).
Heejung Park
(contributions by)Heejung Park is a cultural and developmental psychologist in the Department of Psychology, Scripps College, US. She studies cultural values, identities, and the lived experiences of children, youth, and families across diverse socio-cultural settings and historical periods. She is particularly interested in identifying the challenges and strengths associated with adaptation experiences following social change and migration in an increasingly interconnected world.
Annemieke Milks
(contributions by)Annemieke Milks is a Paleolithic archaeologist, and currently holds a British Academy Postdoctoral Fellowship at the University of Reading, UK. Her work explores the intersection between material culture, skill, and learning. She specializes in weaponry, early wooden artefacts, and Pleistocene childhoods. Annemieke’s work is interdisciplinary, exploring the archaeological record through macro- and micro-imaging of artefacts, morphometrics, experimental archaeology, and ethnography.
Felix Reide
(contributions by)Felix Riede is Professor of Archaeology in the Department of Archaeology and Heritage Studies, Aarhus University, Denmark. He works in a broad and inclusive cultural evolutionary framework. Focused on understanding the interactions between social learning, niche construction, and environmental change, he explores the role of children in human cognitive evolution, and the role of object play as a motor for material culture variation, innovation, and adaptation.
Roman Stengelin
(contributions by)Roman Stengelin completed his PhD in Psychology at Leipzig University, Germany, in 2020, followed by postdoctoral research at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, where he now works as a senior scientist. His research focuses on the development of childhood social cognition and learning, with an emphasis on (cross-)cultural perspectives. He has conducted f ieldwork among Namibian San communities, including the Khwe and Hai||om.
Akira Takada
(contributions by)Akira Takada is Professor in the Graduate School of Asian and African Area Studies at Kyoto University, Japan. He has worked with/among groups of the San of southern Africa (particularly !Xun and ǂAkhoe in Namibia, G|ui and Gǁana in Botswana) since the late 1990s. He has published a number of books and articles, including Hunters Among Farmers: The !Xun of Ekoka (2022), and The Ecology of Playful Childhood: The Diversity and Resilience of Caregiver-Child Interactions among the San of Southern Africa (2020).
Joscha Kärtner
(contributions by)Joscha Kärtner received his PhD from the Department of Culture and Development at the University of Osnabrück, Germany, and is head of the Developmental Psychology Lab and the Center for Learning, Development and Counseling at the University of Münster, Germany. From a dynamic developmental systems perspective, his main research interests include cultural influences on early social, socio-cognitive and socio-emotional development. Besides basic research in these fields, a second emphasis is on developing culturally informed programs and policies for applied developmental science.
- Acosta, L. J. (2023, June 10). Four Colombian children found alive in jungle weeks after plane crash. Reuters. https://www.reuters.com/world/americas/four-missing-colombian-children-found-alive-jungle-sources-2023-06-10/
- Aladsani, H., Al-Abdullatif, A., Almuhanna, M., & Gameil, A. (2022). Ethnographic Reflections of K–12 Distance Education in Saudi Arabia: Shaping the Future of Post-Pandemic Digital Education. Sustainability, 14(16), Article 16. https://doi.org/10.3390/su14169931
- Agliati, A., Vescovo, A., & Anolli, L. (2005) Conversation patterns in Icelandic and Italian people: Similarities and differences in rhythm and accommodation. In L. Anolli, S. Jr Duncan, M. S. Magnusson, & G. Riva (Eds). The hidden structure of interaction: From neurons to culture patterns (pp. 223–235). IOS Press.
- Amir, D., & McAuliffe, K. (2020). Cross-cultural, developmental psychology: Integrating approaches and key insights. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(5), 430–444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.06.006
- Apicella, C., Norenzayan, A., & Henrich, J. (2020). Beyond WEIRD: A review of the last decade and a look ahead to the global laboratory of the future. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(5), 319–329. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.07.015
- Arnold, J. E. (2012). Detecting Apprentices and Innovators in the Archaeological Record: The Shell Bead-Making Industry of the Channel Islands. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 19(2), 269–305. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-011-9108-1
- Barr, R. G., Konner, M., Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. (1991). Crying in !kung San Infants: A Test of the Cultural Specificity Hypothesis. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 33(7), 601–610. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.1991.tb14930.x
- Bengtsson, S. (2014). Faraway, so close! Proximity and distance in ethnography online. Media, Culture & Society, 36(6), 862–877. https://doi.org/10.1177/0163443714531195
- Berl, R. E. W., & Hewlett, B. S. (2015). Cultural Variation in the Use of Overimitation by the Aka and Ngandu of the Congo Basin. PLoS ONE, 10, e0120180. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120180
- Bird, D. W., & Bliege Bird, R. (2000). The Ethnoarchaeology of Juvenile Foragers: Shellfishing Strategies among Meriam Children. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 19(4), 461–476. https://doi.org/10.1006/jaar.2000.0367
- Blurton-Jones, N. G. & Marlowe, F. W. (2002). Selection for delayed maturity: Does it take 20 years to learn to hunt and gather? Human Nature, 13(2), 199–238. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-002-1008-3
- Bock, J. (2002). Learning, life history, and productivity: Children’s lives in the Okavango Delta of Botswana. Human Nature, 13(2), 161–198. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-002-1007-4
- Broesch T., Crittenden, A. N., Beheim, B. A., Blackwell, A. D., Bunce, J. A., Colleran, H., Hagel, K., Kline, M., McElreath, R., Nelson, R. G., Pisor, A. C., Prall, S., Pretelli, I., Purzycki, B., Quinn, E. A., Ross, C., Scelza, B., Starkweather, K., Stieglitz, J., & Mulder, M. B. (2020). Navigating cross-cultural research: methodological and ethical considerations. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 287, 20201245. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.1245
- Broesch, T., Lew-Levy, S., Kärtner, J., Kanngiesser, P., & Kline, M. (2023). A roadmap to doing culturally grounded developmental science. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 14(2), 587–609. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-022-00636-y
- Burger, O., Chen, L., Erut, A., Fong, F. T., Rawlings, B., & Legare, C. H. (2023). Developing Cross-Cultural Data Infrastructures (CCDIs) for research in cognitive and behavioral sciences. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 14(2), 565–585. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-022-00635-z
- Cooperrider, K. (2019, January 23). What Happens to Cognitive Diversity When Everyone Is More WEIRD? Aeon. https://aeon.co/ideas/what-happens-to-cognitive-diversity-when-everyone-is-more-weird
- Crittenden, A. N., Conklin-Brittain, N. L., Zes, D. A., Schoeninger, M. J., & Marlowe, F. W. (2013). Juvenile foraging among the Hadza: Implications for human life history. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 299–304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.04.004
- Deffner, D., Rohrer, J. M., & McElreath, R. (2022). A causal framework for cross-cultural generalizability. Advances in methods and practices in psychological science, 5(3), 25152459221106366. https://doi.org/10.1177/25152459221106366
- Dennis, W. (1940). The Hopi child (pp. xi, 204). Appleton-Century.
- Doyle, L., Brady, A.-M., & Byrne, G. (2009). An overview of mixed methods research. Journal of Research in Nursing, 14(2), 175–185. https://doi.org/10.1177/1744987108093962
- Duranti, A., Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. B. (Eds). (2012). The handbook of language socialization. Blackwell.
- Endicott, K. M., & Endicott, K. L. (2008). The Headman was a Woman: The Gender Egalitarian Batek of Malaysia. Waveland Press, Inc.
- Fargas-Malet, M., McSherry, D., Larkin, E., & Robinson, C. (2010). Research with children: Methodological issues and innovative techniques. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 8(2), 175–192. https://doi.org/10.1177/1476718X09345412
- French, J. C. (2019). The use of ethnographic data in Neanderthal archaeological research. Hunter Gatherer Research, 4(1), 25–49. https://doi.org/10.3828/hgr.2018.3
- Gauvain, M., & Parke, R. D. (2014). Socialization. In Handbook of cultural developmental science (pp. 239–258). Psychology Press.
- Gelman, A., Vehtari, A., Simpson, D., Margossian, C. C., Carpenter, B., Yao, Y., Kennedy, L., Gabry, J., Bürkner, P.-C., & Modrák, M. (2020). Bayesian workflow. arXiv preprint arXiv:2011.01808. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2011.01808
- Gosselain, O. P. (2016). To hell with ethnoarchaeology! Archaeological Dialogues, 23(2) https://doi.org/10.1017/S1380203816000234
- Gudykunst, W. B., & Nishida, T. (1994) Bridging Japanese/North American differences. Sage.
- Haidle, M. N., & Stolarczyk, R. (2020). Thinking Tools. With Cognigrams from Reconstructions and Interpretations to Models about Tool Behavior. Intellectica, 73(2), 107–132. https://doi.org/10.3406/intel.2020.1967
- Hall, J., Gaved, M., & Sargent, J. (2021). Participatory Research Approaches in Times of Covid-19: A Narrative Literature Review. International Journal of Qualitative Methods, 20, 16094069211010087. https://doi.org/10.1177/16094069211010087
- He, A., Greenfield, P. M., Akiba, A. J., & Brown, G. (2022). Why do many parents expect more help from their children during COVID-19? Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology, 3, 100052. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cresp.2022.100052.
- Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., McElreath, R., Alvard, M., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Henrich, N. S., Hill, K., Gil-White, F., Gurven, M., Marlowe, F. W., Patton, J. Q., & Tracer, D. (2005). “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and brain sciences 28, 795–815. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05000142
- Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The Weirdest People in the World?. Behavioural and Brain Sciences 33, 61–81. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
- Hill, N. E. (2021). In search of the individual embedded in context: Applications of the specificity principle. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 75, 101289. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2021.101289
- Hill, K. & Hurtado, A. M. (1996). Ache Life History: The Ecology and Demography of a Foraging People. Aldine de Gruyter.
- Holding, P., Anum, A., van de Vijver, F. J. R., Vokhiwa, M., Bugase, N., Hossen, T., Makasi, C., Baiden, F., Kimbute, O., Bangre, O., Hasan, R., Nanga, K., Sefenu, R. P. S., A-Hayat, N., Khan, N., Oduro, A., Rashid, R., Samad, R., Singlovic, J., Faiz, A., & Gomes, M. (2018). Can we measure cognitive constructs consistently within and across cultures? Evidence from a test battery in Bangladesh, Ghana, and Tanzania. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 7(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2016.1206823
- Hruschka, D. J., Munira, S., Jesmin, K., Hackman, J., & Tiokhin, L. (2018). Learning from failures of protocol in cross-cultural research. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115, 11428–11434. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1721166115
- Jordan, P. (2015). Technology as human social tradition: Cultural transmission among hunter-gatherers. University of California Press.
- Kaplan, H. S., & Bock, J. A. (2001). Fertility theory: The embodied capital theory of life history evolution. In J. M. Hoem (Ed.). International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences (3.3, 155, pp. 5561–5568). Elsevier Science.
- Kärtner, J., Schwick, M., Wefers, H., & Nomikou, I. (2022). Interactional preludes to infants’ affective climax – Mother-infant interaction around infant smiling in two cultures. Infant Behavior and Development, 67, 101715. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2022.101715
- Kievit, R., Frankenhuis, W. E., Waldorp, L., & Borsboom, D. (2013). Simpson’s paradox in psychological science: a practical guide. Frontiers in psychology, 4, 54928. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00513
- Kline, M. A., Boyd, R., & Henrich, J. (2013). Teaching and the life history of cultural transmission in Fijian villages. Human Nature, 24, 351–374. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-013-9180-1
- Kline, M. A. (2015). How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behavioral and Brain sciences, 38, e31. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X14000090
- Kline, M. A. (2016). TEACH: An ethogram-based method to observe and record teaching behavior. Field methods, 29(3), 205–220. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525822X16669282
- Kline, M. A., Shamsudheen, R., & Broesch, T. (2018). Variation is the universal: making cultural evolution work in developmental psychology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373, 20170059. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0059
- Klöter, H. (2004). Language Policy in the KMT and DPP eras. China Perspectives, 2004(6), Article 6. http://journals.openedition.org/chinaperspectives/442
- Konken, L. C., & Howlett, M. (2023). When “home” becomes the “field”: Ethical considerations in digital and remote fieldwork. Perspectives on Politics, 21(3), 849–862. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1537592722002572
- Kramer, K. L. (2021). Childhood Teaching and Learning among Savanna Pumé Hunter-Gatherers. Human Nature, 32(1), 87–114. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09392-x
- Lancy, D. F., Bock, J., & Gaskins, S. (2010). The Anthropology of Learning in Childhood. Rowman Altamira.
- Lancy, D. F. (2008, 2015, 2022). The Anthropology of Childhood: Cherubs, Chattel, Changelings (1st, 2nd, and 3rd ed.). Cambridge University Press.
- Lancy, D. F. (2015). Children as a reserve labor force. Current Anthropology, 56, 545–568. https://doi.org/10.1086/682286
- Lancy, D. F. (2016). Playing with knives: The socialization of self-initiated learners. Child Development, 87, 654–665. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12498
- Lee, J. E. (2024, February 12). More than half of South Koreans reject ‘defector’ labels for North Korean escapees. Radio Free Asia. https://www.rfa.org/english/news/korea/defector-refuegee-escapee-north-korea-south-korea-terminology-07152024154618.html.
- Leighton, D., & Kluckhohn, C. (2013). Children of the People: The Navaho Individual and His Development. In Children of the People. Harvard University Press. https://doi.org/10.4159/harvard.9780674423558
- LeVine, R. A. (2010). The Six Cultures Study: Prologue to a History of a Landmark Project. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41(4), 513–521. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022110362567
- Lew-Levy, S., Pope, S. M., Haun, D. B. M., Kline, M. A., & Broesch, T. (2021) Out of the empirical box: A mixed-methods study of tool innovation among Congolese BaYaka forager and Bondongo fisher–farmer children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 211, 105223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105223
- Lew-Levy, S., Andersen, M. M., Lavi, N., & Riede, F. (2022). Hunter-Gatherer Children’s Object Play and Tool Use: An Ethnohistorical Analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 824983. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.824983
- Lew-Levy, S., Bombjaková, D., Milks, A., Kiabiya Ntamboudila, F., Kline, M. A., & Broesch, T. (2022). Costly teaching contributes to the acquisition of spear hunting skill among BaYaka forager adolescents. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 289(1974), 20220164. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2022.0164
- Lew-Levy, S., Milks, A., Kiabiya Ntamboudila, F., Broesch, T., & Kline, M. A. (2021). BaYaka Adolescent Boys Nominate Accessible Adult Men as Preferred Spear Hunting Models. Current Anthropology, 62(5), https://doi.org/10.1086/716853
- Lupton, D. (2021). Doing fieldwork in a pandemic. https://docs.google.com/document/d/1clGjGABB2h2qbduTgfqribHmog9B6P0NvMgVuiHZCl8/edit#heading=h.ze8ug1cqk5lo
- Mackie, M. E., Surovell, T. A., & O’Brien, M. (2015). Identifying Stone Alignments Created by Adults and Children: A Case Study from a Dukha Reindeer Herder Summer Camp, Khövsgöl Aimag, Mongolia. Sibirica, 14(2), https://doi.org/10.3167/sib.2015.140202
- Ma-Kellams C. (2021). Using true experiments to study culture: Manipulations, measurement issues, and the question of appropriate control groups. Methods in Psychology 4, 100046. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.metip.2021.100046
- Maynard, A. E., Greenfield, P. M., Childs, C. P., & Weinstock, M. (2023). “Social change, cultural evolution, weaving apprenticeship, and development”: Informal education across three generations and 42 years in a Maya community. Applied Developmental Science, 28(1), 82–105. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2022.2151445
- McElreath, R. (2018). Statistical rethinking: A Bayesian course with examples in R and Stan. Chapman and Hall/CRC.
- Milks, A., Lew-Levy, S., Lavi, N., Friesem, D. E., & Reckin, R. (2021). Hunter-gatherer children in the past: An archaeological review. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 64, 101369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaa.2021.101369
- Norenzayan A., & Heine, S. J. (2005). Psychological Universals: What Are They and How Can We Know? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 763–784. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.763
- Ochs, E. (1988). Culture and language development: Language acquisition and language socialization in a Samoan village. Cambridge University Press.
- Ochs, E., Solomon, O., & Sterponi, L. (2005). Limitations and transformations of habitus in Child-Directed Communication. Discourse Studies, 7(4-5), 547–583. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461445605054406
- Odden, H., & Rochat, P. (2004). Observational learning and enculturation. Educational and Child Psychology, 21, 39–50. https://doi.org/10.53841/bpsecp.2004.21.2.39
- Onwuegbuzie, A. J., & Leech, N. L. (2005). On becoming a pragmatic researcher: The importance of combining quantitative and qualitative research methodologies. International journal of social research methodology, 8(5), 375–387. https://doi.org/10.1080/13645570500402447
- Park, H. (2019). A model of identity development: Life-course narratives of North Korean youth resettling in South Korea. Journal of Adolescence, 6, 162–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.08.014
- Pearl, J. (1995). Causal diagrams for empirical research. Biometrika, 82(4), 669–688. https://doi.org/10.1093/biomet/82.4.669
- Pearl, J. (2000). Causality: Models, reasoning, and inference. Cambridge University Press.
- Pearl, J., & Mackenzie, D. (2018). The book of why: the new science of cause and effect. Basic Books.
- Pfundmair M. (2017). Cross-Cultural Experimental Research. In Y. Y. Kim (Ed.) The International Encyclopedia of Intercultural Communication (pp. 1–8). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118783665.ieicc0099
- Ploder, A., & Hamann, J. (2021). Practices of Ethnographic Research: Introduction to the Special Issue. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 50(1), 3–10. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891241620979100
- Portes, A., & Rivas, A. (2011). The adaptation of migrant children. The Future of Children, 21, 219–246. https://doi.org/10.1353/foc.2011.0004
- Pretelli, I., Borgerhoff Mulder, M., & McElreath, R. (2022). Rates of ecological knowledge learning in Pemba, Tanzania: Implications for childhood evolution. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 4, e34. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2022.31
- R Core Team. (2023). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing [Computer software]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
- Ratih, K., Syah, M. F. J., Nurhidayat, N., Jarin, S., & Buckworth, J. (2021). Learning Patterns during the Disruptive Situation in Informal Education: Parents’ Efforts and Challenges in the Adjustment of Progressive Learning. Indonesian Journal on Learning and Advanced Education (IJOLAE), 3(3), Article 3. https://doi.org/10.23917/ijolae.v3i3.15151
- Riede, F., Lew-Levy, S., Johannsen, N. N., Lavi, N., & Andersen, M. M. (2023). Toys as Teachers: A Cross-Cultural Analysis of Object Use and Enskillment in Hunter–Gatherer Societies. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 30(1), 32–63. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-022-09593-3
- Sandberg, H., Sundin, E., & Sjöberg, U. (2022). When ethnographic work turns into distant screen visits: A note on flexible inflexibility during the COVID-19 pandemic. Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood, 23(3), 361–365. https://doi.org/10.1177/14639491221107361
- Schegloff, E. A. (1987). Between micro and macro: Contexts and other connections. In J. C. Alexander, B. Giesen, R. Munch, & N. J. Smelser (Eds). The micro-macro link (pp. 207–234). University of California Press.
- Schegloff, E. A. (2007). Sequence organization in interaction : A primer in conversation analysis (vol. 1). Cambridge University Press.
- Sousa, E. L. (2022). Ethnography with children in times of pandemic: An ethical and methodological reflection. Cadernos de Pesquisa, 52, e09122. https://doi.org/10.1590/198053149122_en
- Stan Development Team. (2021). Stan Modeling Language Users Guide and Reference Manual [Computer software].
- Sullivan, A., Brewis, A., & Wutich, A. (2018). Studying Children’s Cultural Knowledge and Behaviors Related to Environment, Health, and Food: Methods for Ethnoecological Research with Children. Journal of Ethnobiology, 38(2), 276–294. https://doi.org/10.2993/0278-0771-38.2.276
- Takada, A. (2021). Pragmatic reframing from distress to playfulness: !Xun caregiver responses to infant crying. Journal of Pragmatics, 181, 180–195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2021.05.021
- Tannen, D. (2006). Intertextuality in interaction: Reframing family arguments in public and private. Text & Talk, 26(4–5), 597–617. https://doi.org/10.1515/TEXT.2006.024
- Tehrani, J. J., & Riede, F. (2008). Towards an archaeology of pedagogy: Learning, teaching and the generation of material culture traditions. World Archaeology, 40(3), 316–331. https://doi.org/10.1080/00438240802261267
- Thajib, F., Stodulka, T., Haun, H., Meng, T., Sun, W., Masaquiza, S., Arroyo Garcia, B., Junker, M., Petrovic, L., Striegler, B., Weyrowitz, J., Sunderarajan, J., Swastyastu, M., Taek, D. J., Abis, A., Tjizao, D., Kanngiesser, P., Shishala, D., & Liebal, K. (under review). No Safe Distance? Reflections on decolonising large-scale, remote collaborative research during and beyond COVID-19. Ethos.
- Tsuji, S., Amso, D., Cusack, R., Kirkham, N., & Oakes, L. M. (2022). Empirical research at a distance: New methods for developmental science. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 938995. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.938995
- UNHCR. (2023). Figures at a glance. Retrieved October 14, 2023, from UNHCR website: https://www.unhcr.org/figures-at-a-glance.html
- Volpe, C. R. (2019). Digital diaries: New uses of PhotoVoice in participatory research with young people. Children’s Geographies, 17(3), 361–370. https://doi.org/10.1080/14733285.2018.1543852
- Warren, G. (2021). Is There Such a Thing as Hunter-Gatherer Archaeology? Heritage, 4(2), 794–810. https://doi.org/10.3390/heritage4020044
- Watson, A., & Lupton, D. (2022). Remote Fieldwork in Homes During the COVID-19 Pandemic: Video-Call Ethnography and Map Drawing Methods. International Journal of Qualitative Methods, 21, 160940692210783. https://doi.org/10.1177/16094069221078376
- Wefers, H., Krüger, V., Iza Simba, N. B., & Guandinango, Y. A. (2023). Ethical challenges of cross-cultural research – The example of a psychological research project in the Andean context. Journal of Empirical Research on Human Research Ethics, 18(4), 233-245. https://doi.org/10.1177/15562646231181880
- Wefers, H., Schuhmacher, N., Chacón, L. H., & Kärtner, J. (2023). Universality without uniformity – infants’ reactions to unresponsive partners in urban Germany and rural Ecuador. Memory & Cognition, 51(3), 807–823. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-022-01318-x
- Wefers, H., Schuhmacher, N., & Kärtner, J. (2023). Developmental changes in infants’ affective reactivity across cultures. Manuscript Submitted for Publication.
- Wefers, H., Schwarz, C. L., Hernández Chacón, L., & Kärtner, J. (2022). Maternal ethnotheories about infants’ ideal states in two cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 53(6), 603–625. https://doi.org/10.1177/00220221221096785
- West, E. (1992). Children on the plains frontier. In E. West & P. Petrik (Eds). Small Worlds: Children and Adolescents in America, 1850–1950 (pp. 26–41). University Press of Kansas.
- Xu, J. (2019). Learning “Merit” in a Chinese Preschool: Bringing the Anthropological Perspective to Understanding Moral Development. American Anthropologist, 121(3), 655–666. https://doi.org/10.1111/aman.13269
- Xu, J. (2020). The Mischievous, the Naughty, and the Violent in a Taiwanese Village: Peer Aggression Narratives in Arthur P. Wolf’s “Child Interview” (1959). Cross-Currents, 9(1), 180–208. https://doi.org/10.1353/ach.2020.0003
- Xu, J. (2024). “Unruly” Children: Historical Fieldnotes and Learning Morality in a Taiwan Village. Cambridge University Press.
- Yarkoni, T. (2022). The generalizability crisis. Behav Brain Sci, 45, e1. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X20001685
- Yilmaz, K. (2013). Comparison of quantitative and qualitative research traditions: Epistemological, theoretical, and methodological differences. European journal of education, 48(2), 311–325. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejed.12014